PROFESSOR: HI EVERYONE. GOOD MORNING. GOOD TO SEE SO MANY OF YOU HERE
THIS MORNING
AFTER A HOLIDAY WEEKEND. THANK YOU FOR BRAVING THE
FOG AND THE COLD TO BE HERE. WE'RE GOING
TO JUMP RIGHT IN I
THINK. UNLESS YOU HAVE ANY BURNING LOGICAL QUESTIONS I CAN
HELP
WITH QUESTIONS FROM PREVIOUS LECTURES. I'LL BE REVIEWING A FEW THINGS IN THIS LECTURE
SO
HOPEFULLY I'LL ADDRESS THINGS YOU HAVE QUESTIONS ABOUT. BY ALL
MEANS COME BY OFFICE
HOURS, WE'RE HAVING A GOOD TIME THERE IF YOU
WANT TO ASK EXTRA QUESTIONS IN OFFICE HOURS.
BUT ANYTHING, ANYTHING ON COURSE
LOGISTICS I CAN HELP WITH RIGHT NOW. YES?
STUDENT:
DO WE HAVE DISCUSSIONS THIS WEEK?
PROFESSOR: YOU DO HAVE DISCUSSIONS THIS WEEK, CORRECT?
ALL: NO. [LAUGHTER]
PROFESSOR: THAT ANSWERS THAT. RIGHT. OKAY. WHEN YOU HAVE A HOLIDAY
DAY OFF, EVERYTHING
IS CANCELED. SO AMAZINGLY YOU'RE STILL, IT'S BEEN SUCH A
GRADUAL ENTREE
INTO THIS COURSE, WHICH IS NICE. BUT STARTING NEXT WEEK
IT'S GOING TO GET REALLY BUSY
FOR YOU. I WANT YOU TO BE
PREPARED. I DON'T WANT YOU TO BE SURPRISED BY THE PACE ONCE
THIS CLASS ACTUALLY GETS GOING. BUT NO, NO DISCUSSIONS THIS
WEEK, NO LABS THIS WEEK.
SO YOU'RE STILL EASING IN.
OKAY. I SHOULD GET RIGHT IN. I HAVE A LOT OF SLIDES.
CELL
PHONES OFF. INCLUDING MINE.
OKAY. WE'RE TALKING ABOUT INTERSPECIFIC RELATIONSHIPS
PRIMARILY TODAY. RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN SPECIES. AND THIS WILL
BE THE FOUNDATION FOR US FOR
OUR LOOK AT COMMUNITY ECOLOGY. SO
WE WILL SET UP OUR CATEGORIZATION SCHEME
FOR THE TYPES OF
INTERSPECIFIC RELATIONSHIPS THAT EXIST. AND WE'LL FOCUS IN TURN
ON COMPETITION
AND PROVIDE SOME EXAMPLES OF COMPETITION FROM
FIELD STUDIES. WE WILL MODIFY OUR EQUATIONS
FOR LOGISTIC GROWTH
FROM LAST TIME IN SHOWING HOW COMPETITION BETWEEN SPECIES CAN BE
MODELED
MATHEMATICALLY. WE WILL THEN FOCUS ON PREDATION
ANOTHER MAJOR TYPES OF ECOLOGICAL INTERACTION.
AND SUBSUMING UNDER THIS
HERBIVORY, NOT DRAWING A DISTINCTION BETWEEN PREDATION, WHERE
ONE CREATURE CONSUMES ANOTHER CREATURE. AND IN TALKING ABOUT
PREDATION WE'LL TALK A
BIT ABOUT PREDATOR-PREY CYCLES IN THE
CONTEXT OF OUR STUDIES OF POPULATION GROWTH. AND THEN,
YEAH, WE
WILL TALK ABOUT HERBIVORY SEPARATELY FROM THAT. DRAWING OUT THE
SIMILARITIES
MORE THAN DIFFERENCES WITH PREDATION. AND THEN FINISHING MOSTLY
WITH JUST PICTURES
AND STORIES CONCERNING MUTUALISM AND
COMMENSALISM.
BEFORE I REVIEW A COUPLE OF THINGS FROM THE
LAST LECTURE,
LET'S INTRODUCE THE CATEGORIES WE'RE GOING TO BE DEALING
WITH TODAY. IN
OUR CLASSIFICATION OF INTERSPECIFIC INTERACTIONS
YOU CAN CLASSIFY SUCH INTERACTIONS IN VARIOUS
WAYS. AND NONE OF
THEM ARE PERFECT. NONE OF THEM IS PERFECT. WE WILL FOCUS ON
THE
EFFECTS OF THESE INTERACTIONS ON THE PARTICIPANTS AS A MEANS
FOR CLASSIFYING THE TYPE OF INTERACTION.
SO WE WILL DISTINGUISH
COMPETITION FROM PREDATION AND HERBIVORY FROM COMMENSALISM
AND
MUTUALISM. AS I JUST SAID, THE CATEGORIES ARE DISTINGUISHED
BASED ON WHETHER THEY
HAVE A NEGATIVE, A POSITIVE, OR A NEUTRAL
EFFECT ON THE PARTICIPANTS. AND WE'LL
JUST USE A PLUS AND A
MINUS AND A ZERO IF YOU WANT TO INDICATE POSITIVE INFLUENCE,
NEGATIVE INFLUENCE NO INFLUENCE.
AND WITH REGARD TO AN INFLUENCE ON A SPECIES, WHAT
MIGHT I
BE REFERRING TO? A POSITIVE INFLUENCE OR A NEGATIVE INFLUENCE?
INFLUENCING WHAT
ABOUT A SPECIES OR A POPULATION OF AN ORGANISM? NUMBER OF
INDIVIDUALS, IF YOU'RE TALKING
POPULATION, PERHAPS. REPRODUCTIVE SUCCESS,
IF YOU'RE TALKING ABOUT AN INDIVIDUAL, PERHAPS.
RATE OF GROWTH, IF YOU'RE
TALKING ABOUT AN INDIVIDUAL WHICH SHOULD REFLECT THE SUCCESS
OF
THAT INDIVIDUAL AND ULTIMATELY ITS REPRODUCTIVE OUTPUT.
STUDENT: LONGEVITY?
SO YOU HAVE THE IDEA ABOUT THESE POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE EFFECTS.
SO WE'LL START THE FOCUS ON COMPETITION, IN PART BECAUSE
COMPETITION HAS BEEN A MAJOR
FOCUS OF ECOLOGICAL STUDIES SINCE
DARWIN WITH INCREDIBLE DEBATE. AND SWINGS OF THE
PENDULUM IN
TERMS OF THE RECOGNITION OF ITS IMPORTANCE OR ITS DE-EMPHASIS,
IT'S
LACK OF IMPORTANCE IN NATURE. BERKELEY HAS BEEN AN
INSTITUTION THAT HAS FOCUSED A LOT
ON THE STUDY OF COMPETITION.
AND BY ALL ACCOUNTS COMPETITION IS REAL AND OCCURS IN
NATURE.
BUT JUST REALIZE THAT ECOLOGISTS VARY A LOT IN THE DEGREE TO
WHICH THEY CONSIDER
IT AS A MAJOR STRUCTURING FACTOR IN NATURE.
IT HAS A LONG HISTORY OF STUDY OF COMPETITION.
GOING BACK TO
DARWIN, YES. BUT IT REALLY TOOK OFF WITH SOME OF THE WORK OF THE
GAUSE.
IN THE '30S. IN THE 1930S IN PARTICULAR. LABORATORY
STUDIES PRIMARILY THAT LENT THEMSELVES TO
THEORETICAL INSIGHTS.
AND EVEN PHILOSOPHICAL INSIGHTS ABOUT NATURE THAT PROVIDED
FOUNDATIONS FOR LATER STUDY IN BOTH LAB AND FIELD. AND IN
THEORY. SOME OF GAUSE'S ORIGINAL
DATA THERE CLEANED UP A BIT
FOR MORE GENERAL CONSUMPTION. WE CAN LOOK AT THEM HERE. HE'S
LOOKING AT PARAMECIA IN THE LABORATORY, GROWING GROWING TWO DIFFERENT SPECIES OF PARAMECIA
IN ISOLATION AND THEN TOGETHER. DID I ENLARGE MY CURSOR?
NOT SO MUCH, BUT YOU CAN SEE
IT. FOCUSING ON THESE CURVES.
THESE CURVES ARE OF SIGMOID SHAPE, RIGHT? LET'S JUST USE
THIS
OPPORTUNITY TO REVIEW A MINUTE FROM WHAT HAPPENED WAY BACK LAST
WEEK. SO IF
THE LIGHTS COULD COME UP A MOMENT? THANK YOU. LET'S JUST REVIEW A BIT
ON POPULATION GROWTH
AND OUR MODELING WORK FROM LAST TIME. RECALL
THAT IN THE ABSENCE OF RESOURCE LIMITATIONS,
A POPULATION AS A
RESULT OF ITS OWN INTRINSIC CAPACITY TO INCREASE OVER TIME IN
TERMS
OF ITS NUMBERS
WILL INCREASE GEOMETRICALLY OR
EXPONENTIALLY, RIGHT? SO WE REFER TO THIS AS GEOMETRIC INCREASE
OR
EXPONENTIAL INCREASE. AND WE MODELED IT, RIGHT, IT'S IN THE
SHADOWS HERE. AS
DNDT EQUAL TO R, THE INTRINSIC RATE OF INCREASE PER
CAPITA RATE OF GROWTH TIMES N. AND IT
STEEPENS WITH TIME BECAUSE N IS
BECOMING LARGER, SO HAVE A COMPOUNDING OF THE
GROWTH
RATE. JUST LIKE YOU WOULD HAVE A COMPOUNDING OF THE
INTEREST IN THE YOUR BANK ACCOUNT
IF YOU LEFT AN INITIAL SUM IN
THERE. THE GROWTH, THE INTEREST ON YOUR INITIAL SUM YEAR
AFTER
YEAR WILL GROW AS A RESULT OF THE GROWING BASE OF MONEY IN THE
BANK ACCOUNT.
THE SAME WAY A POPULATION WILL GROW, THE
GROWING NUMBER OF INDIVIDUALS WILL PROVIDE
A LARGER BASE. AND R
TIMES N AS A RESULT WILL INCREASE MORE STEEPLY WITH TIME. SO
THAT'S MOST BASICALLY WHAT WE UNDERSTAND WITH GEOMETRIC INCREASE.
AND THERE ARE PLENTY
OF EXAMPLES OF THAT IN THE LAB AND IN NATURE. BUT THIS
CANNOT CONTINUE FOREVER, OF COURSE.
THERE ARE LIMITS TO GROWTH.
AND THESE LIMITS TO GROWTH ACT SUCH THAT POPULATION GROWTH
THROUGH TIME, SAME AXES HERE, IS FREQUENTLY SEEN TO SLOW AND TO
LEVEL OFF. SPEAKING IDEALLY. THINGS ARE OFTEN MUCH NOISIER
THAN THIS. MORE COMPLICATED THAN THIS. STILL MODELABLE
BUT RARELY SO SIMPLE. AND YET
THERE ARE PLENTY OF EXAMPLES.
PARTICULARLY IN LAB SETTINGS IN THIS TYPE OF SIGMOIDAL,
OR SIGMOID GROWTH. THIS IS THE
LOGISTIC CURVE, LOGISTIC MODEL. WHERE AT SOME POINT
HERE, THE EFFECTS OF THE
LIMITATIONS OF THE ENVIRONMENT ACT ON THE GROWING POPULATION
TO
LIMIT ITS NUMBERS IN TIME. WE MODEL THIS WITH THE ADDITION
OF AN EXTRA TERM TO OUR
EXPONENTIAL GROWTH MODEL. RIGHT? AND THAT'S JUST
WITH REFERENCE TO THE CARRYING CAPACITY,
K. WHICH WE CALLED
K. WHICH IS WITH REFERENCE TO THE EXISTING POPULATION UNDER
STUDY,
THE CARRYING CAPACITY IS THE TOTAL NUMBER OF INDIVIDUALS
OF THAT POPULATION IN THAT
ENVIRONMENT LOCALLY AT THAT TIME THAT
CAN BE SUPPORTED. IT'S ALWAYS CONTEXTUAL, TO CARRYING
CAPACITY.
THE CARRYING CAPACITY FOR ANTS ON THE BERKELEY CAMPUS IS SOME
THEORETICAL
NUMBER RIGHT NOW. BUT AT A DIFFERENT SEASON THAT
CARRYING CAPACITY MIGHT CHANGE AND
IT WILL CHANGE. AND 20 YEARS
NOW, GIVEN THE CHANGES IN THE ENVIRONMENT WITH A, WITH
DIFFERENCES IN OTHER ORGANISMS THAT LIVE HERE AND CLIMATE, THE
CARRYING CAPACITY IS LIKELY
TO BE DIFFERENT AGAIN. AND THE
CARRYING CAPACITY OF ANTS HERE IS DIFFERENT FROM THE
CARRYING
CAPACITY OF ANTS ON ANOTHER UNIVERSITY CAMPUS. IN SPACE IT
DIFFERS.
CARRYING CAPACITY IS ALWAYS WITH REFERENCE TO
PARTICULAR PLACES AND PARTICULAR TIMES.
BUT WE CAN MODEL THIS TYPE OF SIGMOIDAL GROWTH WELL WITH
THIS SIMPLE EQUATION. AND WHAT
YOU NEED TO DO IF YOU HAVEN'T
ALREADY IS UNDERSTAND HOW THE EFFECT OF THIS TERM CAME,
MINUS N
OVER K, PRODUCES THIS GRADUAL APPROACH TO AN ASYMPTOTIC APPROACH
TO THE CARRYING
CAPACITY. AND LET ME GIVE YOU, IF YOU HAVEN'T
DONE IT ALREADY, WE HAD A COUPLE OF GEE WHIZ
MOMENTS IN OFFICE
HOURS WHERE PEOPLE STARTED TO SEE JUST HOW THAT WORKS. AND IT
REALLY
IS AN ESTHETICALLY SATISFYING MODEL. I REALLY HOPE YOU WILL
ARRIVE AT THAT UNDERSTANDING.
SO I JUST WOULD LIKE TO SUGGEST
ONE WAY TO DO THAT. SO LET'S ASSUME, LET'S ASSUME
A FIXED
INTRINSIC RATE OF INCREASE PER CAPITA GROWTH RATE OF ONE FOR
SIMPLICITY. LET'S
ASSUME R EQUALS ONE. LET'S ASSUME OUR
CARRYING CAPACITY, LET'S CHANGE IT FROM ANTS TO RABBITS,
JUST FOR FUN, IS A
HUNDRED RABBITS IN THIS SETTING THAT WE'RE TALKING ABOUT. SAY
DOWN
AT THE MARINA, IN THE PARK DOWN IN THE MARINA WHERE THEY
HAVE A BUNCH OF JACK RABBITS
NOW THAT THEY'VE RESTORED THE LAND TO MORE NATURAL CONDITIONS.
A HUNDRED JACK RABBITS
CAN LIVE DOWN THERE AND THEY HAVE AN R OF ONE. WHEN THAT LAND
WAS RESTORED DOWN THERE,
AND I CAN ALSO SEE I'M GOING TO RUN OUT
OF ROOM HERE, WHEN THAT LAND WAS RESTORED
AT THE MARINA, THERE
WAS ONLY ONE RABBIT. AND IF THERE WERE ONLY ONE RABBIT, WHAT
WOULD OUR TERM K MINUS N OVER K BE? IF K EQUALS 100?
99 OVER 100, RIGHT? WHICH IS CLOSE
TO ONE. .99. SO
WHEN THAT TERM IS VERY HIGH LIKE THAT, CLOSE TO ONE, THIS
EQUATION IS BASICALLY GOING TO GO TO RN, THE EXPONENTIAL.
YOU'RE GOING TO SEE EXPONENTIAL
GROWTH WHEN THE POPULATION NUMBER IS
VERY, VERY LOW, LIKE ONE. IF IT GOES UP TO 50, IF
YOU HAVE 50 RABBITS
AFTER A CERTAIN AMOUNT OF TIME, WHAT WILL K MINUS N OVER K EQUAL?
EQUAL A HALF, RIGHT? .5. AND THAT WILL HAVE A HALVING EFFECT
ON R TIMES N WHICH WILL
LEAD TO THIS SLOWING OF GROWTH. YOU CAN
FOLLOW THAT OUT. YOU CAN FOLLOW IT OUT ALL
THE WAY TO 99 WHERE
YOU HAVE THE TERM GO TO 0.01. RIGHT? RADICALLY SLOWING THE
GROWTH
AS IT APPROACHES THE CARRYING CAPACITY THERE. REMEMBER
CARRYING CAPACITY IS 100 SO YOU'RE
ALMOST THERE. OR TO A
HUNDRED ITSELF. AND AT A HUNDRED YOU'RE GOING TO HAVE A HUNDRED
MINUS A HUNDRED IN THE NUMERATOR. RIGHT? SO CHANGE NUMBERS OVER TIME,
ZERO, GROWTH WILL
CEASE. SO YOU CAN SEE THAT. YOU SHOULD BE ABLE TO
IF YOU JUST WORK THROUGH IT A COUPLE OF TIMES. POPULATION
DYNAMICS ARE USUALLY MUCH MORE
COMPLICATED. WE PRESENT THE
SIMPLE CASE INITIALLY. TO BUILD ON. AND MODELERS DO MAKE
MORE
COMPLEX THESE, THE MATHEMATICS HERE TO BETTER FIT THE REALITY OF
NATURAL POPULATION.
BUT WE'LL STOP AT THIS POINT FOR OUR
PURPOSES REALLY IN MODELING THE GROWTH OF INDIVIDUAL
POPULATIONS.
I THINK I MENTIONED THAT IN A PREVIOUS LECTURE THAT SOME OF
WHAT I WAS
DOING WAS DUMBED DOWN. AND SOMEONE ASKED ME
AFTERWARDS ARE YOU REALLY DUMBING IT DOWN
IN HERE? AND I AM DUMBING IT DOWN.
THAT'S, BUT THAT'S THE NATURE OF INTRODUCTORY SCIENCE.
IT'S THE
NATURE OF LEARNING ANY SKILL REALLY. AS YOU LEARN, YOU LEARN THE
BASICS, THE
FOUNDATIONS ON WHICH TO BUILD AND THEN HOPEFULLY AS
YOU BECOME MORE EXPERIENCED, YOU
START TO CHALLENGE AND ERODE
THOSE FOUNDATIONS AS YOU ADVANCE. SO I THINK THAT'S JUST FINE,
WE ARE PRODUCING SOMETHING SIMPLE. AND IN SOME WAYS MAY BE
WRONG BECAUSE THINGS HAVE
ADVANCED BEYOND I DON'T UNDERSTAND THAT.
BUT YOU NEED TO LEARN THE BASICS SO YOU CAN
LEARN HOW THESE
THINGS ARE SIMPLE OR WRONG OR SHOULD BE CHALLENGED.
ALL RIGHT. SO BACK
TO GAUSE. WHEN HE GREW PARAMECIA IN
ISOLATION, THEY DID FOLLOW A ROUGHLY SIGMOID GROWTH CURVE.
BUT
IN GROWING THEM TOGETHER, ONE SPECIES WAS SEEN TO PERSIST WHEREAS
THE OTHER WENT
TO LOCAL EXTINCTION. AND GAUSE INTERPRETED THIS
AS IN TERMS OF A COMPETITIVE SUPERIORITY
OF THE ONE SPECIES IN
RELATION TO THE OTHER. THIS LED HIM TO WHAT BECAME KNOWN AS THE
COMPETITIVE EXCLUSION PRINCIPLE.
THE IMPORTANCE OF THIS ONLY WAS DRAWN
OUT OVER TIME. AND YOU CAN FIND VARIOUS DEFINITIONS
OF IT.
ONE DEFINITION THAT PRODUCES A COUNTERPOINT TO WHAT'S GIVEN IN
YOUR BOOK, IF TWO COMPETING
SPECIES COEXIST IN A STABLE
ENVIRONMENT, THEY DO SO AS A RESULT OF ECOLOGICAL
DIFFERENTIATION.
AND IT'S BEEN A VERY POWERFUL PRINCIPLE IN
STRUCTURING ECOLOGICAL THOUGHT AND ECOLOGICAL
EXPERIMENTS. BUT
PLEASE DO REALIZE THAT THE IMPORTANCE OF IT IN NATURE IS HOTLY
DEBATED.
SO LET'S FOCUS ON COMPETITION. YOU WANT A DEFINITION,
COMPETITION IS AN
INTERACTION BETWEEN ORGANISMS BASED ON A SHARED
REQUIREMENT THAT IS IN LIMITED SUPPLY. YOU
DON'T COMPETE FOR
SOMETHING IF IT'S NOT IN LIMITED SUPPLY. WE DON'T TEND TO
COMPETE FOR OXYGEN, WE HUMAN INDIVIDUALS. MOST PARTS OF THIS
PLANET. RIGHT NOW THERE'S
ENOUGH. AND WE DON'T TEND TO BATTLE
ONE ANOTHER FOR IT. IN SOME AREAS WE DO COMPETE
FOR, SAY,
WATER. WHICH IS IN VERY SHORT SUPPLY, FRESH WATER IN MANY PLACES.
AND
COMPETITION CAN BE REAL AND SOMETIMES FIERCE OR SUBJECT TO --
BUT LET'S DISTINGUISH TYPE
OF COMPETITION. INTERFERENCE
COMPETITION FROM EXPLOITATIVE COMPETITION. WHEN YOU THINK
OF
INTERFERENCE COMPETITION THINK OF A BEHAVIORAL INTERACTION THAT
CAUSES A PHYSICAL INTERACTION.
SO THINK OF DIRECT PHYSICAL
INTERACTIONS BETWEEN INDIVIDUALS. EXAMPLES GIVEN HERE OF
A
COUPLE OF BEETLES VYING FOR POSITION, PERHAPS VYING FOR POSITION
IN RELATION TO
MATING OPPORTUNITIES. LOOKING HERE AT A LION AND
A HYENA. IF YOU THOUGHT CATS AND DOGS
DIDN'T LIKE EACH OTHER,
TRY LIONS AND HYENAS. THEY REALLY DON'T LIKE EACH OTHER. AND
THEY
WILL BATTLE SOMETIMES TO THE END. AND IN A COMPETITIVE
SPIRIT AS A RESULT OF THAT.
USUALLY COMPETITION FOR FOOD.
THEY EAT SIMILAR THINGS. THERE ARE DIFFERENCES THAT ARE VERY
REAL WHERE LIONS FOCUS SOMEWHAT MORE ON THE SOFT TISSUES OF PREY.
THE HYENAS WILL ALSO
CONSUME THE HARD TISSUES, THE BONES. THE
HYENA MIGHT SCAVENGE AFTER A LION HAS MADE
A KILL. THEY'RE,
THEY WILL NOT JUST EAT THE BONES AND HOOVES BUT WILL EAT ALL POO
AND PREVIOUS THINGS THAT THE OTHERS HAVE LEFT BEHIND. THERE ARE
DIFFERENCES THAT DO EXIST.
REAL ECOLOGICAL DIFFERENCES BUT THE
COMPETITION IS REAL ENOUGH THAT WHEN THEY ENCOUNTER EACH
OTHER
THERE'S SOME KIND OF DIRECT CONFLICT. THEY CAN'T GET TOO
CLOSE TO EACH OTHER.
INTERFERENCE COMPETITION, BETWEEN TWO
SPIDERS. BUT WHAT IS COMPETITION FOR? SOME RESOURCE
IN LIMITED
SUPPLY, IT MIGHT FOOD. IT MIGHT BE MATING OPPORTUNITIES. IT
MIGHT BE A SPACE
IN WHICH TO NEST, OR DWELL. IT MIGHT BE WATER. IT MIGHT BE NUTRIENTS.
SO WE SPEAK OF IT
BROADLY, COMPETITION. IN EXPLOITATIVE COMPETITION, THE PARTICIPANTS MAY NOT
ENCOUNTER EACH
OTHER AT ALL. THAT'S WHAT'S MEANT TO BE IMPLIED
BY THIS DIAGRAM WHERE IN THIS CASE TWO CONSUMERS,
A CATERPILLAR
AND A GRASSHOPPER ARE BOTH LEAF CONSUMERS. THEY'RE BOTH
CONSUMING LEAVES
BUT PERHAPS THEY DON'T EVER REALLY PHYSICALLY
ENCOUNTER EACH OTHER. BUT THEY'RE CONSUMING
THE SAME STUFF, SO
LIMITING THE RESOURCE AVAILABLE TO THE OTHER BY THEIR OWN
ACTIVITIES.
AS A RESULT THEY'RE COMPETING BECAUSE THEY'RE
REDUCING THE RESOURCE THAT'S AVAILABLE TO
THE OTHER PARTICIPANT
EVEN THOUGH NOT PHYSICALLY ENCOUNTERING EACH OTHER.
EXPLOITATION OF
THE COMMON RESOURCE, THEY COMPETE.
HERE YOU HAVE A TANGLE OF ROOTS. IN A BANK OF
SOIL. WHAT KIND OF COMPETITION MIGHT YOU SEE HERE?
INTERFERENCE OR EXPLOITATIVE? I HEARD
BOTH. I THINK YOU COULD
SEE BOTH AMONG THESE PLANTS. THE ROOTS MIGHT PHYSICALLY TRY TO
OCCUPY SPACE TO THE EXCLUSION OF OTHER ROOT SYSTEMS. AND THEN
SEEKING SIMILAR NUTRIENTS
OR WATER THEY MAY REDUCE THE AVAILABLE
SUPPLY OF RESOURCES TO ALL THE OTHER PARTICIPANTS.
SO YOU CAN
HAVE INTERFERFENCE AND EXPLOITATION, THEY'RE NOT MUTUALLY
EXCLUSIVE. THIS IS
A GOOD POINT TO INTRODUCE THE CONCEPT OF A
NICHE. HOPEFULLY I'LL HAVE MORE TIME TO LOOK
AT THE HISTORY OF
THIS CONCEPT A LITTLE BIT BECAUSE IT'S VERY INTERESTING,
STRETCHING
BACK TO THE FOUNDER OF OUR MUSEUM HERE. THE MUSEUM
OF VERTEBRATE ZOOLOGY. JOSEPH GRENNEL
WAS ONE OF FIRST PEOPLE TO
FORMALLY DEFINE THE NICHE. AND LATER ECOLOGISTS SUCH AS
CHARLES ELTON AND G. EVELYN HUTCHINSON BUILT UPON THAT AND HOPEFULLY I'LL HAVE MORE TIME
TO TALK ABOUT
THAT WHEN WE TALK ABOUT COMMUNITIES. BUT HERE, THESE CLASSIC
STUDIES PRIMARILY
OF JOSEPH CONNELL FROM THE '60S HELPED TO
ILLUSTRATE THE COMPETITION AND THE IDEA OF
A NICHE. AND THE
DISTINCTION BETWEEN A FUNDAMENTAL NICHE AND A REALIZED NICHE. GOOD
EXAMPLE
FROM YOUR BOOK. YOUR BOOK TENDS TO DO VERY WELL IN THIS
SECTION. SO WE HAVE TWO TYPES
BARNACLES. THEY GROW ON SOLID
SUBSTRATE. PREFLOATING AS LARVAE AND WHEN ANCHORED, FORM
A SHELL
AROUND THEM AND HARDEN ONTO THE SUBSTRATE. THEN THEY'RE
IMMOBILE. IN THIS
AREA, IN THIS REGION WHERE THE OCEAN PICTURED
HERE RISES ALL THE WAY TO A HIGH TIDE LEVEL
AT THIS POINT AND ALL
THE WAY DOWN TO A LOW TIDE LEVEL AT THIS POINT. WE HAVE TWO
SPECIES, TWO GENRE, THE BARNACLE, CHTHAMALUS AND BALANUS GROWING
IN SPECIAL PARTS OF
HABITAT WITH CHTHAMALUS GROWING ALL THE WAY
UP TO THE HIGH TIDE LINE AND BALANUS ALL THE
WAY DOWN TO THE LOW
TIDE LINE. IT WAS POSSIBLE TO STUDY THE FUNDAMENTAL RESOURCES
AND TOLERANCES
OF THESE ORGANISMS TO DIFFERENT ENVIRONMENTAL
FACTORS THROUGH A MANIPULATION EXPERIMENT.
HERE'S WHAT WE JUST
SAW. IT'S THE SAME PICTURE AS BEFORE REALLY, JUST SIMPLIFIED A
LITTLE
BIT. HERE WHAT WE SEE REALIZED IN NATURE IS THIS
CONFIGURATION. WITH CHTHAMALUS OCCUPYING
THE UPPER REGION AND
THE BALANUS THE LOWER. AND NATURE OF THESE ORGANISMS, THE
SPATIAL AND PHYSICAL ENVIRONMENTAL SPACES THAT THESE ORGANISMS
COME TO OCCUPY IN THE
FACE OF NATURAL CONDITIONS. WHEN
CHTHAMALUS WAS REMOVED, THE BLUE BARNACLE FROM THE LOWER
REGION,
WHEN BALANUS WAS REMOVED CHTHAMALUS CAME TO OCCUPY THE REGION.
MOVED INTO THE
TERRITORY THAT WAS PREVIOUSLY OCCUPIED BY BALANUS.
IF YOU REMOVE THE CHTHAMALUS, BALANUS
DOESN'T BUDGE, IT REMAINS
IN THE SAME SPATIAL CONFIGURATION. THROUGH MANIPULATION STUDY
WHERE YOU ACTUALLY REMOVE ONE SPECIES OR THE OTHER, YOU COME TO
SEE THAT THE FUNDAMENTAL
NICHES OF THESE ORGANISMS SEEM TO
DIFFER. THE FUNDAMENTAL NICHE IN THE SENSE THAT THE
BROADEST
POSSIBLE ZONE OF OCCUPATION IN THE ENVIRONMENT THAT AN ORGANISM
CAN OCCUPY
IN THE ABSENCE OF COMPETITION AND OTHER FACTORS, THE
FUNDAMENTAL NICHE OF THE CHTHAMALUS
IS THE ENTIRE REGION BETWEEN LOW AND HIGH TIDE LINES
AS EVIDENCED BY THE REMOVAL EXPERIMENT THAT TOOK BALANUS OUT WHERE IT CAME TO OCCUPY THAT
WHOLE
REGION. BUT BALANUS ON THE
OTHER HAND, ITS REALIZED NICHE IS EQUIVALENT TO ITS FUNDAMENTAL
NICHE. IT DIDN'T BUDGE IN THE ABSENCE OF THIS APPARENT
COMPETITOR. SO WE MUST REALIZE
THAT WHAT WE SEE IN THE NATURAL
WORLD IS A REFLECTION OF DYNAMICS BETWEEN EXISTING
POPULATIONS.
AND IN THE ABSENCE OF COMPETITORS, THE DISTRIBUTION AND ABUNDANCE
OF A PARTICULAR
POPULATION MIGHT DIFFER VERY MUCH. SO I WON'T FOCUS ON THIS FOR LONG. LET'S NOTE
THAT
THIS IS OUR SIMPLE LOGISTICAL MODEL, RIGHT, WITH THIS EXTRA TERM.
IF YOU TAKE THIS EXTRA
TERM OUT YOU'LL HAVE YOUR LOGISTIC MODEL.
WHAT WE WANT TO SHOW HERE IS HOW YOU CAN MODEL
THE COEXISTENCE OF
TWO POPULATIONS OF COMPETITION. THEY'RE CALLED THE
LOTKA-VOLTERRA EQUATIONS.
NAME AFTER AN AMERICAN AND ITALIAN
WHO SIMULTANEOUSLY DERIVED THESE EQUATIONS
FOR THE STUDY OF NATURAL POPULATIONS.
IT'S NAMED AFTER THEM. THEY USED THEM ALSO FOR STUDIES
OF PREDATOR-PREY DYNAMICS. BUT
HERE WE'RE FOCUSED ON COMPETITION. JUST KNOW WHAT WE'RE DOING. YOU
CAN FURTHER DISCUSS
THIS IN DISCUSSION SECTIONS.
I DON'T THINK THIS IS ADDRESSED IN YOUR BOOK. WE NEED A
TERM TO MODEL THE EFFECT OF A SECOND SPECIES ON OUR FIRST
SPECIES, REPRESENTED BY N SUB
1. WE NEED TO CALCULATE THIS
EFFECT OF THE SECOND SPECIES IN TERMS OF THE NUMBERS OF
INDIVIDUALS OF THE FIRST SPECIES. WE DO THAT JUST USING A
COEFFICIENT, A COMPETITION
COEFFICIENT, ALPHA, TO REPRESENT THE
PROPORTIONAL EFFECT THAT A SECOND SPECIES HAS ON THIS FIRST
SPECIES. AND IN THE SAME WAY, WE USE THE COMPETITION
COEFFICIENT DATA TO REFLECT THE EFFECT OF
THE FIRST SPECIES BACK
ON THE SECOND SPECIES TO MODEL THE DYNAMICS OF THAT SECOND
SPECIES
IN TIME. AND THE RELATIVE EFFECTS OF ONE SPECIES ON
ANOTHER MIGHT DIFFER QUITE A LOT. ONE
SPECIES MIGHT HAVE A VERY
STRONG COMPETITIVE EFFECT ON THE SECOND SPECIES WHERE THE OTHER
MAY HAVE A VERY LITTLE EFFECT. AND THAT'S SOMETHING THAT CAN
ALSO BE EMPIRICALLY DERIVED
IN NATURE. YOU CAN CONDUCT STUDIES
IN NATURE TO DERIVE ACTUAL VALUES, REAL WORLD VALUES
FOR THESE
COEFFICIENTS. IF YOU KNOW ONE SPECIES HAS A VERY STRONG
COMPETITIVE EFFECT
ON THE OTHER, YOU WANT TO MODEL THE
COMPETITIVE COEFFICIENT AS VERY HIGH, AS .9 SAY. SO .9
TIMES THE
NUMBER OF INDIVIDUALS IN THAT POPULATION IS GOING TO PRODUCE A
RELATIVELY
LARGE NUMBER TO PLUG INTO THIS EQUATION. AND YOU CAN
FOLLOW THE LOGIC IN THE MODEL OF
THE LOGISTIC CURVE, THE EFFECT
THAT THAT WOULD HAVE. I DON'T WANT TO SPEND TOO
MUCH MORE TIME
ON THIS. IT STRETCHES BEYOND THE LIMITS OF WHAT YOU NEED TO KNOW
HERE
IN TERMS OF OUR MODELING EQUATIONS. I WOULDN'T PROVIDE ANY
FURTHER COMPLEXITY TO THESE
EQUATIONS.
SO LET'S LOOK AT EXAMPLES OF INTERSPECIFIC RELATIONSHIPS IN
NATURE THAT
SEEM TO HIGHLIGHT COMPETITION RELATIONSHIPS. AND
ONE EXAMPLE COMES FROM SOMETHING THAT'S
VERY OFTEN SEEN, IT'S
VERY OFTEN SEEN IN THE ORGANISMS THAT I'M MOST INTERESTED IN,
IN
BIRDS AND MOUNTAINS AS WELL AS LIZARDS. AND OTHER BIRD GROUPS.
THE FACT OF RESOURCE
PARTITIONING, WHERE THE DIFFERENT SPECIES OF
A GROUP, OFTEN A CLOSELY RELATED GROUP,
LIKE A GENUS OF LIZARDS
HERE, ANOLIS LIZARDS OCCUPY DIFFERENT PARTS OF THE HABITAT. IN
THIS CASE DIFFERENT VISIBLE PARTS OF THE HABITAT. A. RICORDII
IS UP IN THE TREES. A. DISTICHUS
IN THE FENCE POST.
IN THE LOWER PORTIONS OF VEGETATION, A. CHRISTOPHEI ARE ALONG
THE TRUNKS OF THE TREES AND ON THE BARK. THINGS LIKE THAT.
THEY'RE OCCUPYING DIFFERENT SPATIAL
PARTS OF THE HABITAT.
THEY'RE DIFFERENTIATING SPACE IN THIS CONCRETE WAY. THEY MAY
NOT
SHOW MUCH EVIDENCE FOR INTERACTION AT ALL. THEY MAY
OCCASIONALLY THROUGH THEIR DUE
LAP OUT AND DO PUSH-UPS AND MAKE
THEMSELVES VISIBLE, BUT PHYSICAL ENCOUNTERS MAY BE VERY
RARE.
SO ECOLOGISTS MIGHT SCRATCH HIS OR HER HEAD AND SAY WHERE'S THE
COMPETITION?
THEY'RE JUST OCCUPYING DIFFERENT SPACES. THEY'RE
DIVIDING THE COMMUNITY IN TERMS OF SPATIAL
RESOURCES. THEY'RE
ALL EATING SIMILAR THINGS. THEY'RE ALL EATING INSECTS THAT THEY
CAN FIT INTO THEIR MOUTH. BUT THEY'RE DOING IT IN DIFFERENT
PARTS OF THE HABITAT AND
THUS NOT INTERACTING. ECOLOGIST NUMBER
TWO MIGHT COME BY AND SAY, THIS SIGNALS THE FACT
THAT COMPETITION
OCCURRED IN EVOLUTIONARY TIME AND LEAD TO THE STRUCTURING OF THIS
GROUP OF SPECIES. IT SIGNALS THE GHOST OF COMPETITION PAST.
AND COMPETITION WAS A
REAL FORCE IN STRUCTURING THIS GROUP. WE
JUST DON'T SEE IT NOW BECAUSE IT HAS ACTED
AND ENSCONCED IN THESE
SPECIES, PARTICULAR NATURAL HISTORY TRAITS THAT THEY EXHIBIT
THEN CAUSES THEM TO DIFFERENTIATE, ENABLES THEM TO COEXIST IN THE
CONSISTENT WITH THE
COMPETITIVE EXCLUSION PRINCIPLE. ANOTHER MAYBE MORE CONCRETE SOURCE OF EVIDENCE
FOR COMPETITION
AND ITS INFLUENCE ON INDIVIDUAL MORPHOLOGY, THE
FORM OF ORGANISMS. MORPHOLOGY, JUST
REFERRING TO THE FORM OF A
CREATURE. THE WAY OF ITS APPEARANCE, THE WAY IT LOOKS. CHARACTER
DISPLACEMENT.
SOME GREAT EXAMPLES OF CHARACTER DISPLACEMENT, THERE ARE TOO MANY OUT THERE.
SOME GOOD ONES
FOR BIRDS AND SOME GOOD ONES FOR MAMMALS THAT I KNOW OF. HERE'S A NICE
ONE FROM
THE GROUP OF FINCHS, DARWIN FINCHES, GEOSPIZA FINCHES FROM THE GALAPAGOS ISLANDS.
ON THIS AXIS YOU HAVE THE DEPTH OF THE BEAK OF THE BIRD MEASURED LIKE
THIS. YOU CATCH
A BIRD IN ITS NET AND HOLD IT IN YOUR HANDS, YOU CAN
MEASURE THE BEAK. IT RANGES FROM
SOMETHING LIKE 7 TO
16 MILLIMETERS. THAT DEPTH OF BEAK IS KNOWN TO BE RELATED TO
THE THINGS THESE BIRDS EAT. THINGS WITH DEEPER BEAKS TEND TO HAVE GREATER
CAPACITY TO CRACK
HARD OBJECTS. AND THE EAT HEAVIER SEEDS. WHEN FULIGINOSA
AND FORTIS OCCUPY ISLANDS WITHOUT
THE OTHER SPECIES PRESENT, SO
WHEN THEY EXIST IN ALLOPATRY IN DISTINCT AREAS, ANOTHER
TERM FOR
LIVING IN DIFFERENT PLACES, THEY HAVE SIMILAR SIZED BEAKS WHEN
THEY'RE
LIVING ALONE WITHOUT THE PRESENCE OF THE OTHER. BUT ON
ISLANDS SUCH AS SANTA MARIA AND SAN
CRISTÓBAL WHERE THEY LIVE
TOGETHER, THEIR BEAK SIZES ARE QUITE DIFFERENT. THE AVERAGE,
THE MEAN SIZE OF THEIR BEAKS, IF YOU RAN A STATISTICAL TEST,
ALMOST CERTAIN TO BE DISTINCT
IN TERMS OF SIZE OF THE BEAKS. IN
THESE TWO POPULATIONS WHEN LIVING SYMPATRICLY TOGETHER.
ASSUMPTION BEING WHEN THE LIVE TOGETHER, THEY NEED TO
DIFFERENTIATE THEIR USE OF RESOURCES.
AND IN DOING SO NATURAL
SELECTION HAS LEAD TO DIFFERENTIATION AND MORPHOLOGY, IN BEAK
MORPHOLOGY IN THE FORM OF
THEIR BILLS RELATED TO THEIR DIFFERENT DIETS. I HAVE INTRODUCED
A FEW
TERMS HERE WHICH YOU'LL USE AGAIN AND AGAIN IN THE
EVOLUTION SECTION. A FAMOUS
OLD PICTURE. I DON'T KNOW WHERE THIS PICTURE CAME FROM,
BUT I SEE IT AROUND FAIRLY OFTEN.
GREAT IMAGE TO ILLUSTRATE A FEW THINGS. I JUST LIKE THE AFRICAN
SAVANNAH. MAYBE NOT
A GREAT IMAGE FOR ALL OF YOU. BUT THIS IS A FASCINATING
LOCATION ON THE PLANET. THIS
TYPE OF SETTING ON THE AFRICAN
PLAIN, OPEN COUNTRY. YOU'LL SEE MY WIFE. I'M SO INTERESTED
IN THAT KIND OF SETTING MORE AND MORE AS I GO ON TO TALKING MORE
AND MORE ABOUT HUMAN
EVOLUTION. HUMANS EVOLVED IN THIS
PART OF THE WORLD AND WE HAVE A LONG HISTORY WITH
THESE KINDS OF
DYNAMICS. IT'S NOT A SITE YOU WANT TO BE HANGINGING AROUND AT A KILL
LIKE THIS
BECAUSE IT'S VERY DYNAMIC AND AGGRESSIVE. WHAT'S HAPPENING
HERE? WHAT
DO THESE ARROWS REPRESENT? BETWEEN THE HYENA AND THE
VULTURE, WHAT HAVE WE? WE HAVE INTERFERENCE
COMPETITION POTENTIALLY. IF THE
HYENA WANTS TO RUN THE VULTURE OFF, TO KILL. OR IF THE
VULTURES
BECOME SO NUMEROUS, THEY CAUSE THE HYENAS PROBLEMS. THAT'S
USUALLY MORE
ONE SIDE WITH THE HYENA RUNNING OFF THE VULTURE. THE
HYENA AND THE ZEBRA, THAT ARROW
COULD ALSO RUN FROM THE VULTURE TO THE ZEBRA, WOULD REPRESENT CONSUMPTION, FOOD
CONSUMPTION
AND PREDATION, RIGHT? BUT WHAT IF THE LIONS ARE ON
THE HORIZON? WHO'S GOING TO SEE THEM
FIRST? PROBABLY A COUPLE
OF THE CIRCLING VULTURES. AND WORD IS GOING TO GET DOWN TO
THE
OTHER VULTURES AND THEN THE HYENAS ARE GOING TO BECOME AWARE.
YOU MIGHT ALSO THINK
OF THAT ARROW BETWEEN THE VULTURE AND THE
HYENA AS SOMETHING COOPERATIVE. WHERE THEY'RE
BOTH CONCERNED
ABOUT LIONS AND THE VULTURE MIGHT BE THE ONE TO SEE THEM FIRST,
OR MAYBE
IT'S THE HYENA BY CHANCE. AND THEY TELL THE OTHER.
SO IT'S A COOPERATIVE ARRANGEMENT
BETWEEN THEM AS WELL. ONE
THING THIS HIGHLIGHTS AS THE DYNAMISM OF THE RELATIONSHIPS,
WHERE
THE RELATIONSHIP CAN BE INTERFERENCE COMPETITION OR
EXPLOITATIVE COMPETITION, IT CAN TURN
INTO COOPERATION. IT CAN
TURN INTO OTHER FORMS IN TIME. AND IT DOES. SO MOVING INTO
SOME OF THESE OTHER TYPES AND THIS IS WHERE I'LL MOVE SOMEWHAT BRISKLY TO GE THROUGH A
LOT OF PICTURES,
STILL ALL VERY IMPORTANT. BUT I WANT TO TRY TO GET THROUGH THIS
MATERIAL.
THE TRUE PREDATORS HAVE MANY HOSTS, USING THAT TERM,
THE TERM HOST IN A FAMILIAR WAY.
AND THE RELATIONSHIP IS LETHAL
IN THE SENSE THAT THE PREDATOR KILLS THE PREY, THE HOST.
AND HAS TO DO SO
AGAIN AND AGAIN TO SUSTAIN ITSELF. AND JUST AN EXAMPLE THERE,
ONE OF
OUR WORLD'S GREAT PREDATORS, THE PRAYING MANTIS, WHICH YOU CAN FIND AROUND BERKELEY. I FOUND
ONE ON TELEGRAPH LAST YEAR. IN A DOORWAY ON TELEGRAPH AVENUE,
RIGHT THERE DOWNTOWN. GREAT CREATURES. YOU CAN HANDLE THEM,
THEY WON'T HURT YOU. ABSOLUTELY
FASCINATING. THEY WILL LOOK
YOU IN THE EYE AND YOU'LL HAVE THIS UNCANNY EXPERIENCE OF
THEIR
STARING YOU IN THE EYE. BUT THE PREY OF PREDATORS GO TO GREAT
LENGTHS TO
AVOID BEING CONSUMED BECAUSE IT'S A LETHAL
ARRANGEMENT. IT DOESN'T SERVE THEM VERY WELL
IN THEIR FITNESS TO DIE. SO A LOT OF
TIMES YOU'LL SEE MORPHOLOGICAL ADAPTATIONS TO AVOID
PREDATION WHERE THEY'RE JUST BEING SPINY AND DIFFICULT TO EAT.
ALL THESE CASES, PORCUPINES
AND FISHERS, ONE OF THE ONLY CONSISTENT
PREDATORS OF PORCUPINES IN NORTH AMERICA, THE FISHER. USUALLY YOU HAVE SOME TYPE OF
PREDATOR THAT CAN OVERCOME THOSE DEFENSES, BUT ATTEMPT TO DEFEND
THEMSELVES IN VARIOUS
WAYS A HOST WILL. APOSEMATISM IS JUST A REFERENCE TO
WARNING COLORATION THAT SIGNALS TO A
PREDATOR THAT I SHOULD NOT
BE EATEN FOR SOME REASON. IN THE CASE OF THESE POISON DART
FROGS IT'S
BECAUSE OF THE TOXINS IN THEIR SKIN. THEY'RE EXTREMELY TOXIC.
AND TO EAT
THEM IS A LEGITIMATE RISK TO THE CONSUMER. AND IT CAN
KILL THE CONSUMER. THEY ADVERTISE
THEMSELVES. THEY LOOK VERY STRIKING
AND THEY STAND OUT. THEY ADVERTISE THEMSELVES.
THAT'S WHAT WE
MEAN IN THIS CONTEXT OF APOSEMATISM. OFTEN ORANGE AND BLACKS OR YELLOWS AND
BLACKS.
IF YOU TURN THE CORNER, YOU WILL SEE THIS, AND YOU'LL
JUMP. YOU JUMP, WELL, MAYBE NOT
IF THEY'RE LITTLE BABIES AND THEY'RE THAT CUTE.
IF THEY'RE GROWN UP AND BIGGER AND YOU SEE
BLACK AND WHITE WHEN YOU TURN THE CORNER IT'LL
GIVE YOU A STARTLE. IT'S
ALMOST INSTINCTUAL REACTION TO THAT TYPE OF WARNING COLORATION,
BLACK ON WHITE. BUT ALSO THINGS LIKE CRYPTICITY AND CAMOUFLAGE,
BLENDING INTO THE SURROUNDINGS,
THAT'S OFTEN A DEFENSE AGAINST
PREDATORS. INSTEAD OF ADVERTISING YOURSELF, YOU TRY TO
BE
OVERLOOKED. THERE'S A FROG IN THERE. PROBABLY AGAINST A BUNCH OF LICHEN.
A DISTINGUISHED
CRYPTICITY FROM CAMOUFLAGE, MAYBE. BECAUSE CRYPTICITY CAN BE IN BEHAVIOR.
IF YOU WATCH A CHAMELEON WALK
ALONG A BRANCH OR A STICK INSECT, THEY'LL OFTEN SHIFT BACK
AND
FORTH, SHIFT BACK AND FORTH AS IF THEY'RE BLOWING IN THE WIND.
THEY'LL MAKE THEMSELVES
LOOK CRYPTIC AS THEY MOVE. CAMOUFLAGE YOU CAN THINK OF IN TERMS
OF COLOR MATCHING
TO THE BACKGROUND.
I'LL GIVE YOU THE SLIDE AND YOU CAN LOOK FOR THE CREATURES THAT MIGHT
BE PRESENT.
SOMETHING ELSE YOU'LL GET IN THE EVOLUTION SECTION, SO I'LL
GLOSS OVER HERE, I THINK
IS FROM THIS PART OF THE BOOK.
MIMICRY. AGAIN, YOUR BLACK AND YELLOW COLORATION SIGNALS
A THREAT. AND IN
THE CASE OF YELLOW JACKETS AND WASPS AND THINGS, BLACK AND
YELLOW USUALLY
SIGNALS THE FACT THAT IT CAN STING YOU. OTHER
ORGANISMS COEXISTING WITH ORGANISMS WITH WARNING
COLORATION MAY
COME TO RESEMBLE THEM. I'LL LET THIS BE DEALT WITH IN
EVOLUTION PRIMARILY.
BUT KNOW THAT THIS TYPE OF MIMICRY CAN BE HONEST OR
DISHONEST. THE MIMICKING ORGANISM
MIGHT ALSO HAVE THE DEFENSES THAT THE
MIMICKED HAS. OR IT MIGHT BE BLUFFING. AND WE DISTINGUISH
MÜLLERIAN FROM BATESIAN MIMICRY TO DISTINGUISH HONEST FROM DISHONEST TYPES OF
MIMICRY.
HERE YOU HAVE A LARVA, LIKE A CATERPILLAR LOOKING AN
AWFUL LOT LIKE A SNAKE. IF A
BIRD IS GOING TO COME IN AND TRY
TO EAT THIS, AND IT FLARES UP, AND LOOKS LIKE A SNAKE,
MAYBE THAT
MIGHT BE A SUFFICIENT STARTLE TO THE BIRD TO FLY AWAY AND LEAVE IT ALONE.
IT'S
BLUFFING, TRYING TO LOOK DANGEROUS WHEN, IN FACT, IT'S RATHER
QUITE HARMLESS.
SO WE DISTINGUISH THESE TWO TYPES OF MIMICRY.
LOOK AT THE PREDATOR-PREY CYCLES AND A COUPLE
OF CLASSIC EXAMPLES. FROM
LAKE SUPERIOR ON ISLE ROYALE. LAKE SUPERIOR FROZE OVER HARD
EARLY IN THE 1900S. AND MOOSE
WALKED ACROSS THE FROZEN ICE AND CAME TO OCCUPY THAT ISLAND
FOR
THE FIRST TIME. IT TURNED OUT TO BE A BOUNTIFUL ISLAND AND
POPULATION NUMBERS
INCREASED QUITE A LOT. THERE WAS A LOT TO
EAT. MOOSE LIKED TO WADE IN THE WATER AND
EAT THE AQUATIC
VEGETATION AND THEY WERE QUITE HAPPY THERE. UNTIL THE LAKE FROZE AGAIN
AND
THE WOLVES ARRIVED. AND THEY CROSSED THE ICE AND JOINED THE
MOOSE ON ISLE ROYALE,
AND SETTING UP A FASCINATING STUDY FOR ECOLOGISTS TO STUDY
PREDATOR-PREY DYNAMICS BECAUSE
WOLF HUNT IN PACKS AND EAT MOOSE. AND THERE'S A LOT OF FOOD THERE.
AND ONE MOOSE KILL
CAN SUSTAIN A FEW WOLVES FOR A LITTLE WHILE.
WHAT ECOLOGISTS WERE PREPARED TO DO IN THIS
INSTANT WAS TO MONITOR THE POPULATION NUMBERS
OF BOTH WOLVES AND MOOSE. AND TO LOOK AT THE
CORRELATIONS BETWEEN
THEM IN TIME. I'LL LET YOU EXPLORE THIS IN YOUR BOOK AS WELL.
BUT NOTE THAT THESE PREDATOR-PREY CYCLES OFTEN APPEAR TO BE ENTRAINED IN SOME WAY. THEY'RE
CORRELATED. AND WHAT BECOMES DIFFICULT TO SORT OUT IS CAUSAL
RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN
POPULATIONS. THEY'RE VERY OFTEN CLEARLY
CORRELATED BUT CAN YOU ASSUME THAT THE CORRELATION
IS ALSO CAUSAL
WHERE THE CHANGES IN THE NUMBERS OF ONE POPULATION ARE CAUSING THE
CHANGE IN THE NUMBERS OF THE OTHER. IN THIS CASE, WHEN MOOSE WERE
SEEN TO INCREASE HERE,
THE WOLVES WERE SEEN TO UNDERGO A PARALLEL
BUT JUST SLIGHTLY STAGGERED IN TIME INCREASE
ALSO TO A HIGH POINT
OF MANY, MANY WOLVES ON THE ONE ISLAND AROUND 1980. THIS CAUSED
APPARENTLY, THIS WAS CORRELATED WITH DECLINE IN MOOSE. AND A
CRASH IN WOLVES. AND YOU
CAN FOLLOW THE CYCLES. AND READ
ABOUT IT AND THINK ABOUT CORRELATION AND CAUSATION.
ANOTHER
FAMOUS EXAMPLE, SORRY ABOUT THE MAMMALS EATING OTHER MAMMALS BUT THESE ARE
THE THINGS I LIKE A LOT.
HERE YOU HAVE A VERY LARGE CAT, CALLED A LYNX AND A FAIRLY
BIG
RABBIT CALLED A SNOWSHOE HARE. AND THE LYNX DEPENDS QUITE A LOT ON THESE BIG
HARES.
THESE ARE THE HARES THAT CHANGE THEIR PELAGE OVER THE COURSE OF THE SEASON SO
THEY'LL BE MORE CAMOUFLAGED IN SUMMER AND ALSO IN WINTER.
BUT THAT DOESN'T KEEP THEM,
OF COURSE, FROM BEING CONSUMED BY THE
LYNX. OH, NO. IT GOT AWAY, I PROMIS. [LAUGHTER]
AT THE ECOLOGICAL LEVEL HERE, THESE NUMBERS WERE
TRACKED ALL THE WAY BACK IN TIME
AS A RESULT OF THE VERY CAREFUL
ACCOUNTING THAT HUNTERS DID IN IDENTIFYING THE NUMBER
OF PELTS THAT THEY
OBTAINED THROUGH THE TRAPPING AND HUNTING SEASONS. THEY HAD TO
REPORT THESE NUMBERS FOR THE LOCAL AUTHORITIES AND APPARENTLY
THEY DID SO WITH SOME FAITHFULNESS.
SO ECOLOGISTS COULD GO BACK
AND MINE THOSE OLD RECORDS TO LOOK AT THE NUMBERS OF INDIVIDUALS
OF THESE POPULATIONS OVER TIME. AND THERE APPEAR TO BE THIS FANTASTIC
ENTRAINMENT
OF NUMBERS OF THE TWO POPULATIONS IN TIME. YET THE
CORRELATION EXISTS BUT THE CAUSAL
RELATIONSHIPS ARE VERY SUBTLE.
AND SOME PEOPLE EVEN SPECULATE THAT THEY'RE RELATED
MORE TO SOLAR
ACTIVITY THAN DIRECTLY TO DYNAMICS ON THE GROUND. AS A RESULT OF
CHANGES
IN SOLAR ACTIVITY YOU COULD HAVE CHANGES IN VEGETATION THAT
PROPAGATE THROUGH THE HERBIVORE
AND THAT INFLUENCE THE PREDATOR.
I GOT YOU FOR TWO MORE MINUTES, GUYS. SETTLE DOWN
FOR A LITTLE BIT LONGER.
SO, YEAH, YOU CAN READ FURTHER ABOUT THAT IN YOUR BOOK.
HERBIVORY CAN BE CONSIDERED IN TERMS OF ITS EFFECTS AS A VERY
SIMILAR PREDATION. BECAUSE
IT BENEFITS ONE PARTICIPANT AND
HARMS THE OTHER. AND HERBIVORY CAN BE ANYTHING FROM
GRAZING BUFFALO OR DUGONGS
AND MANATEES UNDER WATER OR CATERPILLARS AND LEAVES. IT'S
THE
CONSUMPTION OF PLANTS OR ALGAE BY A CONSUMING ORGANISM. YOU'LL
GET A LOT MORE
INTO THIS IN THE BOTANY SECTION. AND PLANTS HAVE
FANTASTIC DEFENSES AGAINST BEING EATN.
YOU'LL HEAR MUCH MORE ABOUT THAT. JUST
NOTE FOR MY PURPOSES SOME OF THE DIVERSITY OF TYPES
OF HERBIVORY.
NECTAR CONSUMPTION OR THE EATING OF FRUIT BY FRUIT BATS
THE USE OF GUNS AND SAPS ARE
ALL TYPES OF PLANT CONSUMPTION OR
HERBIVORY. MUTUALISMS WE CAN SAVE AND TALK ABOUT NEXT
TIME.
THANKS EVERYBODY.